The European Journal of Personality promotes the development of all areas of current empirical and theoretical personality psychology. Welcome to the EJP Blog, the landing page for news related to the European Journal of Personality.

Older people experience loneliness differently. Is that because of their personality?

A post by Thamara Tapia-Munoz

Alice is 72, married, and has three adult children and five grandchildren. Alice has not seen her friends in a long time; her best and only friend, whom she met at school, passed away five years ago. She is retired, in a conformable money situation, and has been at home taking care of the garden and spending time with her family. She and her husband usually play cards and watch movies together. However, she feels lonely very frequently. She struggles to talk about herself, her feelings, the things she worries about and worries very often. Sometimes, she struggles to relax and has felt depressed several times in her life. Her best friend was her confidant. Alice has felt lonely before and remembers her mom was similar to her. When she moved from her parent’s place to go to College, before she met her husband, when her children moved out, and when her parents passed away, she felt lonely. She felt uncomfortable around new people and new places during her entire life and was never part of community activities. School, university, and work provided a natural, safe net in her life.

 

Lily is 71 years old; she is also married and has two children and two grandchildren from her elder son. She is retired and also lives very comfortably. She is part of a literature group where she met people who have become her friends. They have tea together, go to the Theatre, and celebrate everyone’s birthday. She and her husband also have a friend couple that met 30 years ago through Mary’s when she was involved in a parent’s organisation. She feels lonely randomly, but it was hard for her when her kids moved. She went through a depression episode but had pharmacological and talk therapy. She also pushed herself, registered in a volunteer organisation, and started going to the gym. Whenever she feels she needs more activities or to meet new people, she talks to her husband and friends and comes up with a new idea. She rarely feels anxious or stressed; when she does, she actively targets the problem. She remembers both of her parents struggling with being alone, so from a very young age, she learned that actively connecting with others was helpful.

 

 

Loneliness has gotten attention in the past decade, especially since the Covid-19 pandemic. Loneliness can be confused with being alone, but it is not the same concept. As the examples above show, people can be surrounded by others and feel lonely. Loneliness arises when we do not have the social and emotional connections we need. It has been defined as “the negative experience product of the discrepancy between the relationships we need and the ones we have”. Loneliness is considered an evolutionary mechanism. We suffer when socially disconnecting, and that pain triggers us to reconnect or stay connected. We have families, communities, and societies, so we have others. But what happens when we remain in that negative feeling and lack the tools to reconnect with others or make changes?... Then loneliness becomes a severe issue. Loneliness, when it is chronic (very frequent and intense), can have detrimental effects on our mental and physical health. In older adults, loneliness has been associated with depression, dementia, pain, and frailty.

 

 

Loneliness varies from person to person, and those differences might be linked to genes and environmental factors. The evidence has shown that older adults in a partnership with a higher level of education, good health, and good socioeconomic status feel less loneliness than their counterparts of the same age and gender. Women report more loneliness than men, and the association between age and loneliness has a U shape. After fifty years old, loneliness decreases to increase after 80 years old. On the other side, people who are socially isolated and experiencing pain and depression report higher levels of loneliness. However, loneliness is a risk factor for developing clinically significant depression over time.

 

 

Personality traits are defined as a group of core characteristics that interact to create individual variability in psychological functioning. The Big Five theory of personality has described five factors (Extroversion, Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness to experience) that explain individual variation in psychological functioning. These five factors have been validated through different methods, and they have been universally observed. Genetic studies have also shown genetic predisposition and heritability.

 

 

Personality traits and loneliness are genetically associated, but not only that. Personality underlies human behaviours and cognition. Therefore, the way we experience the world and interact with others. Our expectations and beliefs are linked to personality. So, considering that loneliness requires an evaluation between our relationships and the ones we want or need and that reducing loneliness over time is associated with our actions to reduce that gap, our personality might have something to say about the level of loneliness we are experiencing. 

 

 

A recent study analysed 113 studies with people from 12 to 100 years old (Buecker et al., 2020). The results showed that Extroversion and Neuroticism were strongly associated with loneliness and that Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were moderately associated with loneliness. The association between Openness to experience and loneliness was weak and entirely explained by the other traits when they were added together. What we can interpret from those results is that extroverted, emotionally stable, responsible, and reliable people who avoid conflict and get along report lower levels of loneliness. Most of the studies included in the metanalysis were conducted in the general population using cross-sectional methods (similar to a snapshot of a moment in time). So, we need to learn about the role of personality in the level of loneliness that English older people will be experiencing during their later years in life. Understanding the relationship between personality traits and changes in loneliness at an older age might have implications for loneliness interventions and the role of health and social services in supporting those at risk of developing chronic loneliness.

 

 

Our study aimed to analyse the relationship between personality traits and loneliness in older adults, cross-sectionally and longitudinally, considering socio-demographic, economic and health factors, genetic predisposition, and social isolation. We used data from 4,892 older adults 52 years and older from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). ELSA is a nationally representative sample of the English population aged 50 years and above that started in 2002 (Wave 1). Participants are interviewed every two years about different aspects of life and health (sociodemographic characteristics, economic, life expectations, physical health, mental health, psychosocial factors, cognitive functioning, among others), and refreshments (new participants) were included in waves 3, 4, 6, 7 and 9 to maintain representation of the 50 years old group.

 

We analysed ELSA participants from Wave five (2020/2011) until wave nine (2018/2019). The genetic predisposition for loneliness (Polygenic risk scores-PGSs) was derived from genome-wide association studies. Personality using the framework of the Big Five were measured only in Wave 5. Therefore, we observed if the personality was associated with the level of loneliness at the beginning of the observation (wave 5) and if the personality traits were associated with the changes in the level of loneliness over the eight years between wave five and wave 9. We considered age, gender, marital status, educational level, work status, wealth, social isolation, pain, self-perceived health, and depression in the analysis.

 

Our results corroborate previous studies. Extroversion and Neuroticism had the strongest association with the level of loneliness. Conscientiousness, Agreeableness, and Openness to experience associations with loneliness were modified when we considered the other traits and depression. Similar to the examples of Alice and Lily, if we compare people with similar genetic predispositions to loneliness, similar age and health, and same gender, marital status, social isolation, and economic factors, those more sociable or kin to get involved in social interactions and with more emotional stability (the opposite to Neuroticism) were experiencing lower levels of loneliness. Moreover, those characteristics were associated with a higher reduction in the loneliness level over time.

 

In summary, personality traits might be used to detect older people experiencing a higher level of loneliness and those who will remain that way over time. In that direction, although personality (even Neuroticism and Extroversion) explained very little of the changes in loneliness over time, the interplay between a person’s living conditions, disposition towards social life, social connectedness and level of depressive symptoms might help improve mental health and loneliness interventions.

 

 

 

References

 

Aartsen, M., Morgan, D., Dahlberg, L., Waldegrave, C., Mikulionienė, S., Rapolienė, G., & Lamura, G. (2020). Exclusion From Social Relations and Loneliness: Individual and Country-Level Changes. Innovation in Aging, 4(Suppl 1), 712-713. https://doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igaa057.2509

Abdellaoui, A., Chen, H. Y., Willemsen, G., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Verweij, K. J. H., Nivard, M. G., de Geus, E. J. C., Boomsma, D. I., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2019a). Associations between loneliness and personality are mostly driven by a genetic association with Neuroticism. J Pers, 87(2), 386-397. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12397

Abdellaoui, A., Nivard, M. G., Hottenga, J. J., Fedko, I., Verweij, K. J. H., Baselmans, B. M. L., Ehli, E. A., Davies, G. E., Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2018). Predicting loneliness with polygenic scores of social, psychological and psychiatric traits. Genes Brain Behav, 17(6), e12472. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12472

Barreto, M., Victor, C., Hammond, C., Eccles, A., Richins, M. T., & Qualter, P. (2021). Loneliness around the world: Age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences, 169, 110066.

Boomsma, D. I., Willemsen, G., Dolan, C. V., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2005). Genetic and Environmental Contributions to Loneliness in Adults: The Netherlands Twin Register Study. Behavior Genetics, 35(6), 745-752. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-005-6040-8

Bu, F., Steptoe, A., & Fancourt, D. (2020). Loneliness during a strict lockdown: Trajectories and predictors during the COVID-19 pandemic in 38,217 United Kingdom adults. Soc Sci Med, 265, 113521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113521

Buecker, S., Maes, M., Denissen, J. J. A., & Luhmann, M. (2020). Loneliness and the Big Five Personality Traits: A Meta–Analysis. European Journal of Personality, 34(1), 8-28. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2229

Cacioppo, J. T., & Cacioppo, S. (2012). The Phenotype of Loneliness. Eur J Dev Psychol, 9(4), 446-452. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.690510

Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: stability and change.    

              Annu Rev Psychol, 56, 453-484.

Cohen-Mansfield, J., Hazan, H., Lerman, Y., & Shalom, V. (2016). Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(4), 557-576. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610215001532

Dahlberg, L., McKee, K. J., Frank, A., & Naseer, M. (2022). A systematic review of longitudinal risk factors for loneliness in older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 26(2), 225-249. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2021.1876638

Day, F. R., Ong, K. K., & Perry, J. R. B. (2018). Elucidating the genetic basis of social interaction and isolation. Nature communications, 9(1), 2457-2457. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04930-1

Ellis, A., Abrams, M., & Abrams, L. (2009). Personality Theories: Critical Perspectives. In. SAGE Publications, Inc. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452231617

Gerst-Emerson, K., & Jayawardhana, J. (2015). Loneliness as a public health issue: the impact of loneliness on health care utilization among older adults. Am J Public Health, 105(5), 1013-1019. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2014.302427

Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: a theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine : a publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 40(2), 218-227. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8

Steptoe, A., Breeze, E., Banks, J., & Nazroo, J. (2013a). Cohort profile: the English longitudinal study of ageing. Int J Epidemiol, 42(6), 1640-1648. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dys168

Kandler, C., Zimmermann, J., & McAdams, D. P. (2014). Core and Surface Characteristics for the Description and Theory of Personality Differences and Development. European Journal of Personality, 28(3), 231-243. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1952

 

 

 

 

Early Career Researcher Interview - Peter Martin Hähner

A summary of the pitfalls of regression to the mean: Does self-esteem affect job satisfaction?